Skip to main content

Anchoring Bias, or How To Do Self Assessments


There are few assessments as confusing as self-assessments. Is my performance in class at an A, or at a B? Am I 'exemplifying' the 'Learning Habits' expectations, or merely 'meeting' them? What is that even supposed to mean? As if those questions aren't hard enough to answer, there's also the concern about how your teacher will view your assessment of yourself. Some fear that modest self-evaluations may result in a lower final grade by skewing the teacher's thought process toward an unfavourable judgement. Yet going the opposite direction, and grading yourself generously, is also perceived to be harmful. One of my teachers notably quipped that self-assessments are 'a great way to find out who's delusional!'

So, should you evaluate yourself conservatively or should you give yourself the benefit of the doubt? Worry no more, for a finding from behavioural economics may hold your answer.

It's called anchoring, and don't be deceived by the image above, this has nothing to do with ships. Anchoring is a well established cognitive bias that describes the human tendency to rely disproportionately on the first piece of information received when making decisions.

Nobel laureate Daniel Kahneman, along with his research partner Amos Tversky, were among the first researchers to study anchoring. One of their earliest studies involved an arithmetic problem, the answer to which participants had to compute within five seconds. The first group was asked to find 1 x 2 x 3 x 4 x 5 x 6 x 7 x 8, whilst the second group had to solve 8 x 7 x 6 x 5 x 4 x 3 x 2 x 1. Naturally, five seconds was not enough time for most people to solve the problem, so participants made estimates. Although the product of both sets of numbers is clearly the same, those who were presented with the smaller numbers at the beginning of the sequence provided a median estimate of 512, while those whose sequence began with the larger numbers had a median estimate nearly four times higher: 2,250. (The correct answer, unsurprisingly, is much higher than either estimate—40,320.)

In another notable example, participants were randomly shown either the number 10 or the number 65, and then asked to guess the percentage of UN members that were African nations. Participants who saw the number 10 estimated the proportion to be 25% of the UN, while participants who saw 65 estimated it to be 45% of the UN.

The anchoring effect has also been proven to work when the anchoring prompt provided is ridiculous. An experiment was conducted in which half the subjects were asked if Gandhi died at the age of 9 (yes, 9!) and the other half were asked if he died aged 140. Participants were then asked to guess Gandhi's actual age at death. Those primed with the lower number estimated it to be 50, while those primed with the higher number estimated 67. This anchoring bias occurs not only when the anchors are far beyond the range of possibility, but also when test subjects are told about the effect and instructed to prevent any information they've received from influencing their answer.

Relatedly, studies have found the potency of anchoring in negotiations, both from business and political perspectives. Kahneman frequently highlights the benefits of being the first to table a proposal in a negotiation, as this gives one the advantage of implicitly setting the terms for the discussion moving forward. The idea is that even submitting an offer one knows will be taken as outrageous can be helpful in mentally guiding a counterpart toward a favoured outcome.

The mental accessibility of a particular number or figure that is presented in conjunction with a question leads to an unpreventable cognitive bias toward judgements based around—'anchored' by—that number. Our brains engage in something behavioural economists term 'anchoring and adjustment.' Essentially, we tend to start at an implicitly suggested reference point, the anchor, and then adjust upwards or downwards from there. Alas, as seen, these adjustments are rarely sufficient.

What does all of that mean for us as we deal with self-assessments? The question your teacher needs to answer is what grade you deserve. If behavioural economics is anything to go by, your self-assessment has significant power to set an anchor around which the final judgement will be made. With that in mind, being self-effacing may not serve you too well when you grade yourself. But do be careful, or maybe your teacher really will think you're delusional.

Disclaimer: Obviously, anchoring won't work if your grade happens to be based on a strict percentage or number of marks on a test...and if your last four test grades have been Fs, don't expect 'anchoring' your teacher at an A to have any effect! Use this bit of economics at your own risk.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

On Myanmar

This is adapted from a paper I wrote for my Comparative Politics class at Harvard . A Friendless People Thousands have been killed. Hundreds of thousands have fled. They are a people without a home, much less a state. Such is the plight of the Rohingya, whom a UN spokesperson once called the most friendless people in the world. The United Nations High Commissioner for Human Rights has called the situation a “textbook example of ethnic cleansing” (“UN”). This latest wave of state-sponsored violence follows a series of attacks by militants—a small insurgent group, the Arakan Rohingya Salvation Army (ARSA)—in August on Myanmarese military outposts. The brutal military crackdown is ostensibly targeted at insurgents. Yet, it is unmistakably civilians who are bearing the brunt of the violence, with the UN High Commissioner calling the response “clearly disproportionate” and “without regard for basic principles of international law” (“UN”). The approach of ethnic institutionalism has

The Tax Reform India Needs

Image: Tizi A version of this article also appeared in the South China Morning Post's Young Post on Thursday, May 5.  http://yp.scmp.com/news/features/article/103398/india%E2%80%99s-economy-taking-right-steps-goods-and-service-tax India’s current prime minister, Narendra Modi, was elected in 2014 on his economic credentials and the promise that he would bring to the nation the same prosperity he brought, as chief minister, to his home state of Gujarat.  The keystone of Modi’s economic policy is a nationwide goods-and-service tax (GST), intended to streamline the myriad state and central-government levies currently encumbering business in the country.   Under the prevailing tax regime, multiple layers of tax are imposed on the same good, and interstate trade is complicated by varying tax rates between states. Transporting goods across state borders can entail long waiting times to resolve tax matters, and the plethora of obscure charges leaves ample room for co

Where Lurks the Next Crisis?

Photo: SCMP Optimism flows freely through the markets today. Asset prices are at record highs: investors are happier than ever to throw money at barely profitable technology companies, debt issuances from dubious companies and volatile governments, and anything blockchain related—from Bitcoin to Ethereum to Insanecoin, which does actually exist.     Most observers think that the coin mania is a bubble waiting to burst. But bears have been calling the whole market a bubble for a while, predicting an end to the years-long economic recovery and stock market boom. The old joke goes that economists have called seven of the last two recessions. It still rings true. Doomsayers have been convinced of a market crash since the post-recession recovery had barely gotten on its legs. But with things as they are today, are we on a path toward crisis?   I will not engage in the perilous business of trying to predict when the next economic downturn or market tumble will occur. Any such p